The Neural Secrets of Narration

Autori

  • Stefano Calabrese Università degli Studi di Modena e Reggio Emilia

Parole chiave:

Default Mode Network; Endocrinologia, Costruzione dell’intreccio narrativo; Teo-ria della Mente; Cervello sociale; Salienza.

Abstract

Il Default Mode Network (DMN) è il circuito delle regioni cerebrali deputate al racconto attivo o passivo e alla condivisione delle esperienze. Consentendoci di simulare esperienze e usare i ricordi per costruire storie ipotetiche su come le persone potrebbero comportarsi, è un’abilità fondamentale ai fini della costruzione di un aggregato sociale in quanto permette di prefigurare le conseguenze delle nostre azioni e di riflettere sulle intenzioni degli altri, simulando le loro esperienze per anticiparne il comportamento. Elaboriamo finzioni la cui accuratezza è misura delle nostre competenze sociali, in quanto più l’immaginazione si approssima alla realtà sociale, meglio negoziamo i nostri mondi sociali. In ogni caso, il responsabile di tali operazioni è il DMN, che include le strutture frontali e parietali del cervello lungo la linea mediana, i lobi temporali laterali e mediali e la corteccia parietale laterale. Questo contributo analizza il DMN in tutti i suoi aspetti, sia nella costruzione degli intrecci narrativi che nell’allestimento di scenari emozionali credibili e immersivi, che hanno esiti addirittura endocrini (ossitocina e cortisolo).

Riferimenti bibliografici

Baron-Cohen, Simon. La scienza del male. L’empatia e le origini della crudeltà. Trad. it., Raffael-lo Cortina, 2012.

Baumgartner, Thomas, et al. “Oxytocin shapes the neural circuitry of trust and trust ad-aptation in humans.” Neuron, vol. 58, 2008, pp. 639-50.

Bouizegarene, Nabil, et al. “Narrative as active inference: An integrative account of the functions of narratives.” Psyarxiv Preprint, 2020, pp. 1-55.

Brockington, Guilherme, et al. “Storytelling increases oxytocin and positive emotions and decreases cortisol and pain in hospitalized children”, PNAS, vol. 118, n. 22, 2021, pp. 1-7.

Djikic, Maja, Oatley, Keit, and Moldoveanu, Mihnea C. “Reading other minds: Effects of literature on empathy”. Scientific Study of Literature, vol. 3, n. 1, 2013, pp. 28-47.

Fauconnier, Gilles, and Turner, Mark. The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. Basic Books, 2002.

Figueiredo, Tiago, and Mattos, Paulo. “Disentangling the phenomenology of mind-wandering.” Journal of Attention Disorder, vol. 26, n. 4, 2021, pp. 502-07.

Fingelkurts, Andrew A., Fingelkurts, Alexander A., and Kallio-Tamminen, Tarja. “Trait lasting alteration of the brain default mode network in experienced meditators and the experiential selfhood.” Self and Identity, vol. 15, n. 4, 2016, pp.1-13.

Green, Melanie C., and Brock, Timothy C. “The role of transportation in the persua-siveness of public narratives.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 79, n. 5, 2000, pp. 701-21.

Grodal, Torben. Immagini-corpo. Cinema, natura, emozioni. Trad. it., Diabasis, 2014.

Hsu, Chun-Ting, et al. “The magical activation of left amygdala when reading Harry Potter: an fMRI study on how descriptions of supra-natural events entertain and en-chant.” PloS one, vol. 10, n. 2, 2015, pp. 1-15.

Kidd, David Comer, and Castano, Emanuele. “Reading literary fiction improves theory of mind.” Science, vol. 342, n. 6156, 2013, pp. 377-80.

Kurby, Christopher A., and. Zacks, Jeffrey M. “The activation of modality-specific rep-resentations during discourse processing.” Brain and Language, vol. 126, n. 3, 2013, pp. 338-49.

Jacobs, Arthur M., and Willems, Roel M. “The fictive brain: neurocognitive correlates of engagement in literature.” Review of General Psychology, vol. 22, n. 2, 2018, pp. 147-60.

Mar, Raymond A., and Oatley, Keith. “The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience.” Perspectives on Psychological Science, vol. 3, n. 3, 2008, pp. 173-92.

Mehl‐Madrona, Lewis, and Mainguy, Barbara. “Neuroscience and narra-tive.” Anthropology of Consciousness, vol. 33, n. 1, 2021, pp. 1-17.

Meyer-Lindenberg, Andreas, et al. “Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine.” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 12, n. 9, 2011, pp. 524-38.

Nijhof, Annabel D., and Willems, Roel M. “Simulating fiction: individual differences in literature comprehension revealed with fMRI.” PLoS One, vol. 10, n. 2015, e0116492.

Rafetseder, Eva, Cristi‐Vargas, Renate and Perner, Josef. “Counterfactual reasoning: De-veloping a sense of ‘nearest possible world’.” Child Development, vol. 81, n. 1, 2010, pp. 376-89.

Ramstead, Maxwell J.D., Veissière, Samuel P.L. and Kirmayer, Laurence J. “Cultural af-fordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of at-tention.” Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 7, n. 1090, 2016, pp. 1-21.

Raymond, Catherine, et al. “Salivary cortisol levels and depressive symptomatology in consumers and nonconsumers of self-help books: a pilot study.” Neural Plasticity, Ar-ticle ID 3136743, 2016.

Russell, Georgina, and Lightman, Stafford. “The human stress response.” Nature Reviews Endocrinology, vol. 15, n. 9, 2019, pp. 525-34.

Schacter, Daniel L., and Addis, Donna R. “The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 362, n. 1481, 2007, pp. 773-86.

Seth, Anil K., Keisuke Suzuki, and Critchley, Hugo D. “An interoceptive predictive cod-ing model of conscious presence.” Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 2, n. 395, 2012, pp.1-16.

Sharon, Tanya, and. Woolley, Jacqueline D. “Do monsters dream? Young children’s un-derstanding of the fantasy/reality distinction.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 22, n. 2, 2004, pp. 293-310.

Smallwood, Jonathan. “Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: a process–occurrence framework for self-generated mental activity.” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 139, n. 3, 2013, pp. 519-35.

Spreng, R. Nathan, and Grady, Cheryl L. “Patterns of brain activity supporting autobio-graphical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the default mode network.” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 22, n. 6, 2010, pp. 1112-23.

Strawson, Galen. “Against narrativity.” Ratio, vol. 17, n. 4, 2004, 428-52.

Subbotsky, Eugene, and Slater, Elizabeth. “Children’s discrimination of fantastic vs real-istic visual displays after watching a film with magical content.” Psychological Reports, vol. 112, n. 2, 2011, pp. 603-9.

Tamir, Diana I., et al. “Reading fiction and reading minds: The role of simulation in the default network.” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, vol. 11, n. 2, 2016, pp. 215-24.

Tylén, Kristian, et al. “Brains striving for coherence: Long-term cumulative plot for-mation in the default mode network.” Neuroimage, vol. 121, 2015, pp. 106-14.

Weed, Ethan, et al. “Theory of Mind in adults with right hemisphere damage: What’s the story?. Brain Lang, vol. 113, n. 2, 2010, pp. 65-72.

Xu, Jiang, et al. “Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narra-tive comprehension.” Neuroimage, vol. 25, n. 3, 2005, pp. 1002-15.

Dowloads

Pubblicato

01-02-2023

Come citare

Calabrese, S. (2023). The Neural Secrets of Narration. ENTHYMEMA, (31), 319–337. Recuperato da https://riviste.unimi.it/index.php/enthymema/article/view/18654

Fascicolo

Sezione

Saggi
Ricevuto 2022-09-09
Accettato 2022-12-29
Pubblicato 2023-02-01